Leap Day Special: 50% off Family and Teacher Subscriptions!

To celebrate the quirkiness of the leap day, we’re doing a very special offer – just from 29 Feb 2016 until 1 Mar 2016!

Leap years are funny things. Did you know, for instance, that in
Ireland and the United Kingdom when it was expected that men would always ask women to marry them and not the other way around, there was a tradition that it was acceptable for
women to ask men to marry them on Leap Year’s Day?

PostcardLeapYearBeCarefulClara1908An OpenSTEM subscription provides free access to all our base PDF Resources for an entire year! This is many megabytes of awesome materials for you to use, full of colourful text and images. New PDFs are added all the time.

To make use of this limited offer, simply go to the special Leap Day page, or use the LEAPDAY coupon code when checking out one of the aforementioned subscriptions in the store. You will need to specifically add either the Private Family or One Teacher subscription to your cart.

You can also take a peek at what our different resources look like on our Curriculum Samples page.

Junior Primary Students Build Stonehenge Model

Just a few weeks ago junior primary students did the Building Stonehenge Activity, as part of our Integrated History/Geography Program for Primary.

Seville Road State School on Brisbane’s south-side kindly sent us a photo to show you. This class used wooden blocks they happened to have, other classes use collected cardboard boxes.

Year 1-3 Building Stonehenge Activity
Year 1-3 Building Stonehenge Activity (Photo: Seville Rd State School)

Our materials are designed to provide a more engaging learning experience for students as well as teachers. Here, students are examining different types of calendars and ways of measuring time. Stonehenge is given as an example of a solar calendar. This leads naturally into a discussion of solstices, equinoxes and seasons.

Learning New Skills Faster | Washington Post

https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/wonk/wp/2016/02/12/how-to-learn-new-skills-twice-as-fast/

How to level up twice as quickly.

The short version of the research outcome is that when you are learning new skills, just repeating the exact same task is not actually the most efficient. Making subtle changes in the task/routine speeds up the learning process.

Considering our brains work and learn like neural nets (actually neural nets are modelled off our brains, but anyhow), I’m not surprised: repeating exactly the same thing will strengthen the related pathways. But the real world doesn’t keep things absolutely identical, so introducing small changes in the learning process will create a better pattern in our neural net.

Generally, people regard “dumb” repetition as boring, and I don’t blame them. It generally makes very little sense.

The researchers note that too much variation doesn’t work either – which again makes sense, if you think in neural net context: if your range of pathways is too broad, you’re not strengthening pathways very much. Quantity rather than quality.

So, a bit of variety is good for learning!

School Starting Age: Age vs Outcomes

This is such an important topic to raise and discuss – many countries are sending their little tiddlywinks to school earlier, and Australia has done this too. But does it actually improve outcomes? The following article from David Whitebread at Cambridge University already dates back to 2013. Noting several studies, he indicates that sending kids to school at a younger age might not improve outcomes at all: the studies across many countries found that there is no long-term benefit in terms of (for instance) literacy outcome, and there are distinct disadvantages.

Being involved in developing classroom programs based on the Australian Curriculum, it struck us early on what high demands are placed on little five year olds. Particularly if this does not yield long-term benefits in terms of educational and well-being outcomes, I think we need to consider this more. We make our materials as engaging and fun as possible anyhow, but when our national Curriculum prescribes certain things, students and teachers are “required to deliver” and that can create a lot of pressure.

http://www.cam.ac.uk/research/discussion/school-starting-age-the-evidence

Earlier this month the “Too Much, Too Soon” campaign made headlines [in the UK] with a letter calling for a change to the start age for formal learning in schools. Here, one of the signatories, Cambridge researcher David Whitebread, from the Faculty of Education, explains why children may need more time to develop before their formal education begins in earnest.

In the interests of children’s academic achievements and their emotional well-being, the UK government should take this evidence seriously.  — David Whitebread

In England children now start formal schooling, and the formal teaching of literacy and numeracy at the age of four.  A recent letter signed by around 130 early childhood education experts, including myself, published in the Daily Telegraph  (11 Sept 2013) advocated an extension of informal, play-based pre-school provision and a delay to the start of formal ‘schooling’ in England from the current effective start until the age of seven (in line with a number of other European countries who currently have higher levels of academic achievement and child well-being).

This is a brief review of the relevant research evidence which overwhelmingly supports a later start to formal education. This evidence relates to the contribution of playful experiences to children’s development as learners, and the consequences of starting formal learning at the age of four to five years of age

There are several strands of evidence which all point towards the importance of play in young children’s development, and the value of an extended period of playful learning before the start of formal schooling. These arise from anthropological, psychological, neuroscientific and educational studies.  Anthropological studies of children’s play in extant hunter-gatherer societies, and evolutionary psychology studies of play in the young of other mammalian species, have identified play as an adaptation which evolved in early human social groups. It enabled humans to become powerful learners and problem-solvers. Neuroscientific studies have shown that playful activity leads to synaptic growth, particularly in the frontal cortex, the part of the brain responsible for all the uniquely human higher mental functions.

In my own area of experimental and developmental psychology, studies have also consistently demonstrated the superior learning and motivation arising from playful, as opposed to instructional, approaches to learning in children. Pretence play supports children’s early development of symbolic representational skills, including those of literacy, more powerfully than direct instruction. Physical, constructional and social play supports children in developing their skills of intellectual and emotional ‘self-regulation’, skills which have been shown to be crucial in early learning and development. Perhaps most worrying, a number of studies have documented the loss of play opportunities for children over the second half of the 20th century and demonstrated a clear link with increased indicators of stress and mental health problems.

Within educational research, a number of longitudinal studies have demonstrated superior academic, motivational and well-being outcomes for children who had attended child-initiated, play-based pre-school programmes. One particular study of 3,000 children across England, funded by the Department for Education themselves, showed that an extended period of high quality, play-based pre-school education was of particular advantage to children from disadvantaged households.

Studies have compared groups of children in New Zealand who started formal literacy lessons at ages 5 and 7. Their results show that the early introduction of formal learning approaches to literacy does not improve children’s reading development, and may be damaging. By the age of 11 there was no difference in reading ability level between the two groups, but the children who started at 5 developed less positive attitudes to reading, and showed poorer text comprehension than those children who had started later. In a separate study of reading achievement in 15 year olds across 55 countries, researchers showed that there was no significant association between reading achievement and school entry age.

This body of evidence raises important and serious questions concerning the direction of travel of early childhood education policy currently in England. In the interests of children’s academic achievements and their emotional well-being, the UK government should take this evidence seriously.

Reprinted with permission under a Creative Commons License (CC BY-NC-SA).